2021/03/18

The Selfish Gene: 40th Anniversary Edition (Oxford Landmark Science): Dawkins, Richard: 9780198788607: Amazon.com: Books

The Selfish Gene: 40th Anniversary Edition (Oxford Landmark Science): Dawkins, Richard: 9780198788607: Amazon.com: Books

The Selfish Gene: 40th Anniversary Edition (Oxford Landmark Science) 4th Edition
by Richard Dawkins  (Author)
4.6 out of 5 stars    3,374 ratings

As influential today as when it was first published, The Selfish Gene has become a classic exposition of evolutionary thought. Professor Dawkins articulates a gene's eye view of evolution - a view giving centre stage to these persistent units of information, and in which organisms can be seen as vehicles for their replication. This imaginative, powerful, and stylistically brilliant work not only brought the insights of Neo-Darwinism to a wide audience, but galvanized the biology community, generating much debate and stimulating whole new areas of research. Forty years later, its insights remain as relevant today as on the day it was published.

This 40th anniversary edition includes a new epilogue from the author discussing the continuing relevance of these ideas in evolutionary biology today, as well as the original prefaces and foreword, and extracts from early reviews.

Oxford Landmark Science books are 'must-read' classics of modern science writing which have crystallized big ideas, and shaped the way we think.
eTextbook 
from AUD 10.86
Audiobook
AUD 0.00
---
Editorial Reviews
Review

Reviews for 30th Anniversary Edition:


"Dawkins first book, The Selfish Gene, was a smash hit. Best of all, Dawkins laid out this biology-some of it truly subtle-in stunningly lucid prose. (It is, in my view, the best work of popular science ever written.)" --New York Review of Books

"This important book could hardly be more exciting." --The Economist

"The sort of popular science writing that makes the reader feel like a genius." --New York Times

"Who should read this book? Everyone interested in the universe and their place in it." --Jeffrey R. Baylis, Animal Behavior

"This book should be read, can be read, by almost everyone. It describes with great skill a new face of the theory of evolution."--W. D. Hamilton, Science

"The presentations are remarkable for their clarity and simplicity, intelligible to any schoolchild, yet so little condescending as to be a pleasure to the professional." --American Scientist

About the Author

Richard Dawkins, Emeritus Fellow of New College, Oxford, is one of the most influential science writers and communicators of our generation. He was the first holder of the Charles Simonyi Chair of the Public Understanding of Science at Oxford, a position he held from 1995 until 2008, and is Emeritus Fellow of New College, Oxford. His bestselling books include The Extended Phenotype (1982) and its sequel The Blind Watchmaker (1986), River Out of Eden (1995), Climbing Mount Improbable (1996), Unweaving the Rainbow (1998), A Devil's Chaplain (2004), The Ancestor's Tale (2004), and The God Delusion (2007). He has won many literary and scientific awards, including the 1987 Royal Society of Literature Award, the 1990 Michael Faraday Award of the Royal Society, the 1994 Nakayama Prize for Human Science, the 1997 International Cosmos Prize, and the Nierenberg Prize for Science in the Public Interest in 2009.
Product details
Publisher : Oxford University Press; 4th edition (August 1, 2016)
Language : English
Paperback : 544 pages

More about the author
› Visit Amazon's Richard Dawkins Page
Richard Dawkins
 Follow
Biography
Richard Dawkins taught zoology at the University of California at Berkeley and at Oxford University and is now the Charles Simonyi Professor of the Public Understanding of Science at Oxford, a position he has held since 1995. Among his previous books are The Ancestor's Tale, The Selfish Gene, The Blind Watchmaker, Climbing Mount Improbable, Unweaving the Rainbow, and A Devil's Chaplain. Dawkins lives in Oxford with his wife, the actress and artist Lalla Ward.


Very poor Neutral Great
Customer reviews
4.6 out of 5 stars


Top reviews
Top reviews
Top reviews from the United States
Cogitus
5.0 out of 5 stars Much More Than Advertised
Reviewed in the United States on January 5, 2017
Verified Purchase
My Copy: Oxford Univ. Press: 30th Anniversary Edition (Hardbound)

There is much more to "The Selfish Gene" than is advertised, even in the most glowing of its reviews. In fact, I (not a biologist, but fascinated by evolution ever since "dinosaurs" and the first high school biology course) have been vaguely aware of this book since its initial wave of rave reviews many years ago, but never bothered to read it because as it was advertised its theme(s) always seemed pretty obvious. But something recently piqued my curiosity again, not sure what that was now, and after reading the prefatory material online I finally decided to take a look.

After reading quickly through the first 3 chapters, it became apparent that there was a great deal more underlying the book than was overtly presented, that it was not just an over-extended, over-simplified, over-popularized, metaphorical presentation .... but rather that its metaphorical treatment is painstakingly faithful to an elaborate, closely-reasoned, even rigorous, scientific underpinning. At which point, I stopped reading and began again from the beginning, first the prefatory material, then from page 1, this time more slowly and more carefully, taking care to appreciate and reflect on all the markers of the underlying basis and their implications.

This is a wonderful book, even beautiful in many respects, from its initial beginning (at the "beginning") with the purely chemical/physics "evolution" of the primordial soup (cast suggestively in the form of biological evolution); to the consequent continuity with the creation of "replicators", elementary "survival" cells, genes, and the beginnings of life forms; to the important distinction between genes and individuals, as genes and their "survival vehicles" (the first cells and "us", for example); to the nicely extended notion of "gene" itself, required by underlying scientific reality; to a clear presentation of the conflict between Darwinian and "group" selection and evolution; to the nature of evolution, operating (in distinct ways) in terms of both genes and individuals, aka both genes and "their" survival vehicles, aka both chemical/physics and biological evolution; to genetic kinship and its very special selective and social implications; ... ; to the delicious End Notes to the 1st eleven chapters, which provide much supporting and fascinating material.

"The Selfish Gene" goes on to clarify not only its expressed subject, the nature and genesis of Selfishness and Altruism, but to make clear the error, scope, and source of various (idealistic, and often political) arguments and ideas centered around group selection fallacies, including the genesis of (ill-conceived) "group-beneficial", cooperative "functions" vs. (individual) evolutionarily stable strategies (ESS) and kinship. It also sheds light on many other commonly-posed questions, among them: the fundamental "reason" for the 50:50 sex ratio (despite the number of different breeding strategies observed for male competitors); the driving source of the natural variability upon which (continuing) evolution depends; the variety and shadings of competing "strategies", which can be both conceived and advantageous, clustered around a given regard (partly on account of environmental inconstancy), one incidental, unintended but important, implication of which is that this is itself an evolutionary driving source of the natural variability upon which (continuing) evolution operates; .... and NOT so commonly posed: that "In its long journey down the generations therefore, an [ANY] average gene will spend approximately half its time sitting in male bodies, and the other half sitting in female bodies", and thus genes will generally contribute positively to both sexes, sometimes in very different ways, and that, indeed, many "purely male / purely female" effects pass (unexpressed) through many bodies of the opposite sex; and much, much more.

Beautifully written and packed with wonderful insights, "The Selfish Gene" is not only well-worth the read, but will amply reward the reader in proportion to the thoughtfulness and reflection with which they read it. In fact, there is so much food for thought in the story-lines and examples (e.g., the fig, "lichenization", and organelle endosymbiosis) provided in "The Selfish Gene", that one must often stop and consider, at length and at leisure, the questions which it provokes or which Dawkins rhetorically poses.

I will, however, amend Dawkins' wonderful characterization of "us" (Preface to the First Edition, p. xxi): “We are survival machines --- robot vehicles blindly programmed to preserve the selfish molecules known as genes. This is a truth which still fills me with astonishment. Though I have known it for years, I never seem to get fully used to it.” ...... by grafting it to my own previous synopsis, with the result:

"We are Conditioned-Reaction Engines [built on Basic Senses + Unconditioned Reflexes (among them innate Kantian "Categories", instincts, emotions, etc.)] built as Gene-Survival "Machines" [genetically "programmed" to serve the "interests" of our genes] = Pavlo-Kantian Conditioned-Reaction, Darwin-Dawkins Gene-Survival Automatons.
Read less
83 people found this helpful
Helpful
Report abuse
Lucas Rugar
5.0 out of 5 stars One of life's must-reads
Reviewed in the United States on December 26, 2019
Verified Purchase
This book will completely change the way you see people, animals, and all of their interactions. Dawkins says in this anniversary edition of the book that he regrests naming it The "Selfish" Gene because people go into it believing that the thesis of the book revolves around an inevitability of the most selfish genes, and by asoociation, creatures surviving. But a gene being "selfish" is more about it's survival, and "selfishness" in a gene does not mean that the being, composed of millions of genes, is "selfish".

Anwyays, Dawkins is the greatest scientific writer because he is able to break down extremely intricate topics so that the reader can understand. He's a master of using analogies. For instance; he explains a gene's relationship to chromosomes by comparing them to pages in a book of a library. Once he makes these abstract-like concepts more digestible, you're then able to follow along and delve into what it is he really wants to explain about them.

Some parts you might still have to re-read, but even if only 90% of the book sticks, it's well-worth it.
18 people found this helpful
Helpful
Report abuse
TRR Sailboat
5.0 out of 5 stars a population of purely altruistic individuals is not "evolutionarily stable" because a mutant gene for selfishness will spread like wildfire, and vice versa
Reviewed in the United States on September 16, 2018
Verified Purchase
This book thoroughly convinced me of its central claim--that genes, not organisms, are the self-replicating entities upon which natural selection acts. The evidence the book presents (for evolution) is overwhelming, and left me with the uncomfortable feeling that there is little room left for supernatural explanations.

Animal behaviors, both on an individual level and on a species level, are analyzed in terms of what happens to the genes "for" those behaviors, and various predictions are made that turn out to be correct. For example, a population of purely altruistic individuals is not "evolutionarily stable" because a mutant gene for selfishness will spread like wildfire, and vice versa: a population of purely selfish individuals is vulnerable to mutant genes for cooperation. Therefore some "stable" ratio must exist, where any increase in either selfishness or altruism gets punished by natural selection.

Overall it's pretty readable but I found the mathematics surrounding the relatedness of insects to be very difficult, although the explanation for the existence of sterile worker ants is fascinating.
18 people found this helpful
Helpful
Report abuse
dbcopper
2.0 out of 5 stars I would love to give this a higher rating.
Reviewed in the United States on August 31, 2019
Verified Purchase
This book was meant for smarter readers than I ... the subject fascinates me and this is the guy to know about on this subject. In some other world, a perfect one we do not live in, I would want all this information in a "Readers Digest" version. I guess that will never happen and I guess too that I will never understand all that I read, but the elements which make us who we are comprise a very interesting topic and if you have more patience than I, do give this book a try. I simply ran out of time and concentration. Not the authors fault .... (hmmmm, or is it?) Am I just being selfish???
13 people found this helpful
Helpful
Report abuse
Mick Boyce
3.0 out of 5 stars "Selfish Gene" model is not a scientific theory
Reviewed in the United States on January 17, 2020
Verified Purchase
It is a metaphor that many people, including some scientists, have mistaken for a theory. To be a scientific theory, a theory must make testable predictions, and the idea of "selfish genes" makes no testable predictions. It is empty as a theory. For further explanation of this, see Professor Denis Noble's work, including his book "Dance to the Tune of Life". Noble was a thesis advisor for Dawkins, and Noble was the first scientist to mathematically model the human heart on a computer (tubes and punchcards, this was WAY back in the day, folks!). Noble's work led to the pacemaker. Noble and Dawkins have had an ongoing disagreement about evolution.
Evolutionary theory has moved on since Dawkins wrote this book, but Dawkins has not budged. Nevertheless, I recommend Dawkins book because he is a very good writer and this is a great introduction to the science of DNA and evolution. But don't believe for a moment that Dawkins has the final word! Move on to Denis Noble and Robert Trivers. Trivers is a fascinating guy - most of the ideas in The Selfish Gene are originally his, and his paper on altruism and evolution is freely available and a real stunner.
8 people found this helpful
Helpful
Report abuse
See all reviews
Top reviews from other countries
J. lee
5.0 out of 5 stars Must read for anyone interested in genetics
Reviewed in the United Kingdom on February 10, 2019
Verified Purchase
As a biologist, I'm surprised it took me until after I'd finished my biology masters to read this. The book explains some quite complex ideas in a way that a non-science background reader should understand, and Dawkin's writing style was, for me, very engaging and interesting (which isn't easy with some of the topics he writes about here). If you're interested in biology or genetics, I'd recommend it, and if not, I think this book could spark that interest
21 people found this helpful
Report abuse
Amazon Customer
1.0 out of 5 stars Worst copy; best book
Reviewed in India on November 24, 2018
Verified Purchase
I don't know who packed this particular copy for me. A leftover.. within two-three readings, all parts will become separate pieces. The person must have saved good ones for shops and this types of copies for fools like me. The book is a very good one, I know. The copy bad one.
Customer image
45 people found this helpful
Report abuse
Luke
5.0 out of 5 stars An Easy Listen - Audiobook Recommended Over Paper Book
Reviewed in the United Kingdom on March 20, 2016
Verified Purchase
(Note: This review is for the audiobook version)

This book is required reading for my degree at university. As someone who rarely reads and struggles to keep up concentration on a book, I decided (on the recommendation of a lecturer) to buy this audiobook version. I was not at all disappointed. it is read by Dawkins himself and this enables a greater understanding of the text than you could ever get from just reading the book. You can easily tell which elements of his argument make him passionate, and which he felt simply had to be included. Another advantage of this is the placement of footnotes. Having been in discussions with friends about this book, I noted that some found arguments hard to follow because so much of what Dawkins says that is important is contained in footnotes and endnotes. In the audiobook, these are slotted into the text in logical places, preceded by Dawkins saying loudly 'endnote/footnote'.

The only issues with this are it does take a long time (it's well over 16 hours) so you may want to have a good place to sit to listen to it. If you're a heavy commuter this will be perfect for you. The other issue is (for me at least) this cannot be played in a CD player, it has to be played on a computer or other device (e.g. an MP3 player).

In terms of the book and its contents, again, I heartily recommend the selfish gene. Whether an undergraduate, expert in the topic or simply curious about the natural world, this book will be a thrill from start to finish.
Read less
26 people found this helpful
Report abuse
James
5.0 out of 5 stars If this is the standard Richard Dawkins has I will surely read all of his books
Reviewed in the United Kingdom on September 18, 2015
Verified Purchase
This book has added so much light to my view of the world. It has helped build a better picture of the truth about humanity and nature. You will likely find yourself reflecting on the past and becoming more aware in the present and future situations. Really this is a book that builds on the Darwinian theory of evolution. The title most certainly does not fully reflect the content and this is well explained by Richard Dawkins in the book.

While the book as a whole is very interesting and the way which Richard Dawkins writes, the simple and complex logic is beautiful. One of the brilliant examples is when he explains evolutionary stable strategy (ESS) in the human context. Saying that there are 3 kinda of people, the fool, the cheater and the grudger. Really thought provoking and a book worth reading not just once.

If this is the standard Richard Dawkins has I will surely read all of his books.
17 people found this helpful
Report abuse
Wayne Wilmot
5.0 out of 5 stars Important Work
Reviewed in the United Kingdom on January 17, 2019
Verified Purchase
As a retired scientist/engineer I was enthralled with this ground breaking book. I am embarrassed that I did not read it before but I usually read specialist books or fiction. I was guided to it by the most unlikely source: Love Island UK's Jamie and Camilla's conversation about best non-fiction. Well done you two.
6 people found this helpful
Report abuse

The Evolution of Human Sexuality Symons, Donald: Books

Amazon.com: The Evolution of Human Sexuality (9780195029079): Symons, Donald: Books

The Evolution of Human Sexuality 1st Edition
by Donald Symons  (Author)
4.0 out of 5 stars    16 ratings
-
Author Donald Symonds examines the differences between men and women in sexual behavior and attitudes, concluding that these differences are innate and that it is impossible to achieve identical sexualities in males and females. A central theme of this book is that, with respect to sexuality, there is a female human nature and a male human nature, and these natures are extraordinarily different, though the differences are to some extent masked by the compromises heterosexual relationships entail and by moral injunctions. Men and women differ in their sexual natures because throughout the immensely long hunting and gathering phase of human evolutionary history the sexual desires and dispositions that were adaptive for either sex were for the other tickets to reproductive oblivion.

This book is organized as follows: Chapter One introduces basic evolutionary concepts, and Chapter Two considers the special difficulties in applying these concepts to human beings. Since evolutionary analyses of human sexuality traditionally emphasize changes that occurred in the female - the capacity for orgasm and the loss of estrus - Chapters Three and Four respectively deal with these matters. The basic components of sexual selection - intrasexual competition and sexual choice - are taken up in Chapters Five and Six. Chapter Seven, about the desire for sexual variety, emphasizes male sexuality, and Chapter Eight integrates much of the earlier material in arguing that among all peoples sexual intercourse is understood to be a service or favor that females render to males. In Chapter Nine, the hypothesis that male sexuality and female sexuality differ by nature is tested with two independent kinds of evidence. Chapter Ten recapitulates the book's major themes.
--
Includes Tax, if applicable
Available instantly
---
Editorial Reviews
Review

"A classic. I have used it again and again in both senior level classes and graduate seminars."-- Professor Pierre L. Van Den Berghe, University of Washington

About the Author
Donald Symons is at University of California, Santa Barbara.
Product details
Publisher : Oxford University Press; 1st edition (February 5, 1981)
Language : English
Paperback : 368 pages
Customer Reviews: 4.0 out of 5 stars    16 ratings

Customer reviews
4.0 out of 5 stars
---
Top reviews from the United States
Dr(PhD)Wes
5.0 out of 5 stars If you can still find this book, get it.
Reviewed in the United States on May 21, 2017
Verified Purchase
This book is a landmark for the initial scientific assault on what I call "rolethink"--what Steven Pinker called the Standard Sociological Mode of the Mind, in How the Mind Works. .Rolethink has governed the U.S. from its inception and Europe from start of the Industrial Revolution. The machine takes in like cogs and outputs one size fits all.
4 people found this helpful
--
VEL – The Contemporary Heretic
5.0 out of 5 stars The Founding Work of the Modern Evolutionary Psychological Study of Sexuality
Reviewed in the United States on September 20, 2011
Verified Purchase
Research over the last three decades in the field which has become known as evolutionary psychology has focussed disproportionately on mating behaviour. Geoffrey Miller (1998) has even argued that it is the theory of sexual selection rather than that of natural selection which, in practice, guides most research in the field.

This does not reflect merely the prurience of researchers. Rather, given that reproductive success is the ultimate currency of natural selection, mating behaviour is, along perhaps with parental investment, the form of behaviour most directly subject to selective pressures.

Almost all of this research ultimately traces its ancestry to 'The Evolution of Human Sexuality' by Donald Symons. Indeed, much of it was explicitly designed to test claims and predictions formulated by Symons himself.

For example, in his discussion of the age at which women are perceived as most attractive by men, Symons argues that, if human evolutionary history were characterised by fleeting one-off sexual encounters, this would be the age of greatest female fertility (i.e. mid-twenties). However, if men evaluate women for the purposes of more lasting long-term unions, then men should be maximally attracted to women of the highest 'reproductive value' (a term borrowed from demography and population genetics) – in other words, those at the beginning of their reproductive careers, such that, by entering a long-term relationship with the woman at this time, a male is able to monopolise her entire lifetime reproductive output (p189).

Evidence subsequently collected from sources such as 'lonely heats advertisements' and surveys (e.g. Kenrick and Keefe 1992) has found that men perceive women in their late-adolescence (e.g. mid- to late-teens) as most attractive, confirming Symons' intuitive impression that it was women of maximal 'reproductive value' who were perceived as most attractive.

Support has even emerged for some of Symons' more speculative hunches.

For example, one of Symons' two scenarios for the evolution of concealed ovulation, in which he professed "little confidence" (p141), was that this had evolved so as to impede male mate-guarding and enable females select a biological father for their offspring different from their husbands (p139-141).

Consistent with this theory, studies have found that women's mate preferences vary throughout their menstrual cycle in a manner compatible with a 'dual' or 'mixed' mating strategy, preferring males evidencing a willingness to invest in children at most times, but, when at their most fertile, preferring characteristics indicative of genetic quality (e.g. Penton-Voak et al 1999). Meanwhile, in a questionnaire distributed via a women's magazine, Bellis and Baker (1990) found that women engaged in affairs do indeed report engaging in 'extra-pair copulations' (EPCs) at times likely to coincide with ovulation.

Interestingly, Symons even anticipated some of the mistakes evolutionary psychologists would be led into.

For example, he warns that researchers in modern western societies may be prone to overestimate the importance of female choice as a factor in human evolution, because, in their own societies, this is a major factor, if not the major factor, in determining marriage and sexual and romantic relationships (p203).

However, in ancestral environments (i.e. what evolutionary psychologists now call the 'EEA') arranged marriages were likely the norm, as they are in most cultures across the world (p168).

Thus, Symons concludes, "there is no evidence that any features of human anatomy were produced by intersexual selection" (p203) – i.e. by female mate choice, like, for example, the peacock's tail.

[Indeed, the human anatomical trait in humans that perhaps shows the most evidence of being a product of intersexual selection is a female one, namely the female breasts, since the latter are, unlike the mammary glands of most other mammals, permanently present from puberty on, not only during lactation, and composed primarily of fatty tissues, not milk: Møller 1995; Manning et al 1997; Havlíček et al 2016.]

Instead, "human physical sex differences are explained most parsimoniously as the outcome of intrasexual selection" – i.e. male-male competition, especially fighting, as evidenced by the substantially greater upper-body muscle mass of adult males as compared to females.

This was, however, a warning almost entirely ignored by a subsequent generation of researchers before being forcefully reiterated by Puts (2010).

Homosexuality as a 'Test-Case'
An idea of the importance of Symons's work can be ascertained by comparing it with contemporaneous works addressing the same subject-matter.

Edward O Wilson's  On Human Nature  was first published only a year before 'The Evolution of Human Sexuality'. Yet Wilson's chapter on sex bears little resemblance to the subject matter of modern evolutionary psychology and the latter portion of the chapter is devoted to introducing a now faintly embarrassing theory of the evolution of homosexuality which has subsequently received no empirical support (see Bobrow & Bailey 2001).

In contrast, Symons's own treatment of homosexuality is innovative. It is also characteristic of his whole approach and illustrates why 'The Evolution of Human Sexuality' has been described by David Buss as "the first modern treatise on evolutionary psychology proper" ( Handbook of Evolutionary Psychology : p251).

Rather than viewing all behaviours as necessarily themselves adaptive, Symons instead focuses on admittedly non-adaptive (or even maladaptive) behaviours, not because he believes them to be adaptive, but rather because they provide a unique window on the evolved psychology underlying them.

Accordingly, Symons does not concern himself with how homosexuality evolved, implicitly viewing it as a rare and maladaptive malfunctioning of normal sexuality. Instead, he uses homosexuality as a case-study and window on the nature of male and female sexuality as it manifests itself when freed from the constraints imposed by the conflicting demands of the opposite sex.

On this view, the rampant promiscuity manifested by many homosexual men (e.g. 'cruising' and 'cottaging' in bathhouses and public lavatories) reflects the universal male desire for sexual variety when freed from the constraints imposed by the conflicting desires of women – a desire that, although obviously reproductively unproductive among homosexuals themselves, nevertheless evolved because it enhanced the reproductive success of heterosexual men by motivating them to attempt to mate with multiple females and thereby father multiple offspring.

In contrast, burdened with pregnancy and lactation, women's 'potential reproductive rate' is more tightly constrained than that of men. They therefore have little to gain reproductively by mating with multiple males, since they can generally gestate only one offspring at a time.

It is therefore notable that, among lesbians, there is little evidence of the sort of rampant promiscuity common among gay men. Instead, lesbian relationships seem to be characterized by much the same features as heterosexual coupling (e.g. long-term mate bonds).

The similarity of heterosexual coupling to that of lesbians, and the striking contrast with that of male homosexuals, suggests that it is women who exert decisive influence in dictating the terms of heterosexual coupling.

Thus, Symons reports, "there is enormous cross-cultural variation in sexual customs and laws... yet nowhere in the world do heterosexual relations begin to approximate those typical of homosexual men"; leading him to conclude, "heterosexual relations are structured to a substantial degree by the nature and interests of the human female" (p300).

This conclusion, of course, is contrary to the feminist assumption that it is men who dictate the terms of heterosexual coupling and for whose exclusive benefit such relationships are structured.

It also suggests that most men are ultimately frustrated in achieving their sexual ambitions to a far greater extent than are most women. "The desire for sexual variety," he writes, "dooms most human males to a lifetime of unfulfilled longing" (p228).

One is reminded of Camille Camille Paglia's description of men as "sexual exiles who wander the earth seeking satisfaction, craving and despising, never content" and in whose anguished longing "there is nothing... for women to envy" ( Sexual Personae : p19).

Criticisms of Symons's Use of Homosexuality as a Test-Case
There is, however, a potential problem with Symons's use of homosexual behaviour as a window onto the nature of male and female sexuality. The whole analysis rests on a questionable premise – namely that homosexuals are, their preference for same-sex partners aside, otherwise similar, if not identical, to heterosexuals of their own sex in their psychology and sexuality.

Symons defends this assumption, arguing "there is no reason to suppose that homosexuals differ systematically from heterosexuals in any way other than their sexual object choice" (p292).

Indeed, in some respects, Symons sees even "sexual object choice" as analogous among homosexuals and heterosexuals of the same sex.

For example, he observes that, unlike women, both homosexual and heterosexual men tend to evaluate prospective mates primarily on the basis their physical appearance and youthfulness (p295). Thus, in contrast to the failure of periodicals featuring male nudes to attract a substantial female audience (see below), Symons notes the existence of a market for gay pornography parallel in most respects to heterosexual porn – i.e. featuring young, physically attractive models in various states of undress (p301).

This, of course, contradicts the feminist notion that men are led to 'objectify' women only due to the portrayal of the latter in the media.

Instead, Symons contends, "pictures of attractive women are used to sell products because these pictures appeal to men (and perhaps women as well), not the other way around", concluding "that homosexual men are at least as likely as heterosexual men to be interested in pornography, cosmetic qualities and youth seems to me to imply that these interests are no more the result of advertising than adultery and alcohol consumption are the result of country and western music" (p304).

However, this assumption of the fundamental similarity of heterosexual and homosexual male psychology has been challenged by David Buller in his book,  Adapting Minds .

Buller cites evidence that male homosexuals are in some respects 'feminized' in aspects of their behaviour. Indeed, one of the few consistent early correlates of homosexuality is gender non-conformity in childhood and some evidence (e.g. digit ratios, the 'fraternal birth order effect') suggests that the level of prenatal exposure to masculinizing androgens (e.g. testosterone) in utero affects sexual orientation.

As Buller notes, although gay men seem, like heterosexual men, to prefer youthful sexual partners, they also appear to prefer sexual partners who are, in other respects very masculine. Thus, Buller observes, "the males featured in gay men's magazines embody very masculine, muscular physiques, not pseudo-feminine physiques" (Adapting Minds: p227) and indeed seem in most respects similar in physical appearance to the male models, pop stars, actors and other 'sex symbols' and celebrities fantasised about by heterosexual women and girls.

How then are we to resolve this apparent paradox?

One possible explanation that some aspects of the psychology of male homosexuals are feminized but not others – perhaps because different parts of the brain are formed at different stages of prenatal development, at which stages the levels of masculinizing androgens in the womb may vary.

Indeed, there is even evidence that homosexual males may be hyper-masculinized in some aspects of their physiology. For example, it has been found that homosexual males report larger penis-sizes than heterosexual men (Bogaert & Hershberger 1999).

This then may be because, "if it is supposed that the barriers against androgens with respect to certain brain structures (notably those concerned with homosexuality) lead to increased secretion in an effort to break through, or some sort of accumulation elsewhere… then there may be excess testosterone left in other departments" ( Born Gay: The Psychobiology of Sex Orientation : p80).

Another possibility is that male homosexuals actually lie midway between heterosexual men and women in their level of masculinization. They therefore appear relatively feminine only when compared with other (i.e. heterosexual) men.

Compared to women, on the other hand, they may be relatively masculine, as reflected in the male typical aspects of their sexuality focussed upon by Symons.

Interestingly, this latter interpretation suggests the rather disturbing possibility that, freed from the restraints imposed by women, heterosexual men would be even more indiscriminately promiscuous than their homosexual counterparts!

Pornography as a "Natural Experiment"
For Symons, fantasy represents another window onto sexual and romantic desires. Like homosexuality, fantasy is, by its very nature, unconstrained by the conflicting desires of the opposite sex (or indeed by anything other than the imagination of the fantasist).

Symons later collaborated in an investigation into sexual fantasy by means of a questionnaire (Ellis and Symons 1990). However, in the present work, he investigates fantasy indirectly by focusing on "the natural experiment of commercial periodical publishing" – i.e. pornographic magazines (p182).

In many respects, this approach is preferable to a survey because, even in an anonymous questionnaire, individuals may be less than honest when dealing with a sensitive topic such as their sexual fantasies. On the other hand, they are unlikely to regularly spend money on a magazine unless they are genuinely attracted by its contents.

Before the internet age, such magazines, largely featuring female nudes, commanded sizeable circulations. However, their readership (if indeed 'readership' is the right words, since there was typically little reading involved) was almost exclusively male.

In contrast, there was little or no female audience for magazines containing pictures of naked males. Instead, magazines marketed towards women (e.g. fashion magazines) contain, mostly, pictures of other women.

Indeed, when, in the 1970s, attempts was made, in the name of feminism, to market magazines featuring male nudes to a female readership, one such title, 'Viva', abandoned publishing male nudes after just a few years then went bust a few years later, while the other, 'Playgirl', although it did not entirely abandon male nudes, was notorious, as a consequence, for attracting a readership composed in large part of homosexual men.

Symons concludes, "the notion must be abandoned that women are simply repressed men waiting to be liberated" (p183).

Romance Literature
Unfortunately, however, there is notable omission in Symons's discussion of pornography as a window into male sexuality – namely, he omits to consider whether there exists any parallel genre that offers equivalent insight into the female psyche.

Later writers on the topic have argued that romance novels (e.g. Mills and Boon, Jane Austin), whose audience is as female as pornography's is male, represent the female equivalent of pornography, and that analysis of the the content of such works provides insights into female mate preferences parallel to those provided into male psychology by pornography (e.g. Kruger et al 2003; Salmon 2004; see also  Warrior Lovers: Erotic Fiction, Evolution and Female Sexuality ).

Female Orgasm as Non-Adaptive
An entire chapter, Chapter Three ("The Female Orgasm: Adaptation or Artefact"), is devoted to rejecting the claim that the female orgasm represents a biological adaptation.

This is perhaps excessive but at least conveniently contradicts the claim of some critics of sociobiology such as Stephen Jay Gould that the field is 'ultra-Darwinian' or 'hyper-adaptionist' and committed to the misguided notion that all traits are necessarily adaptive.

[Incidentally, Symons also rejects the theory that the menopause is adaptive (p13), a theory which has subsequently become known as the 'grandmother hypothesis'. His discussion of rape also implicitly favours the theory that rape is a by-product of the greater male desire for commitment free promiscuous sex, rather than the product of a specific rape adaptation in males (see Palmer 1991; and  A Natural History of Rape ).]

In contrast, Symons champions the thesis that the female capacity for orgasm is a simply non-adaptive by-product of the male capacity to orgasm, the latter of which is of course adaptive.

On this view, the female orgasm (and clitoris) is, in effect, the female equivalent of male nipples (only more fun!).

Certainly, Symons convincingly critiques the notion, popularised by Desmond Morris among others, that the female orgasm functions as a mechanism to enhance 'pair-bonding' between couples.

However, subsequent generations of evolutionary psychologists have developed less naïve models of the adaptive function of female orgasm.

For example, Geoffrey Miller argues that the female orgasm functions as an adaptation for 'mate choice' ( The Mating Mind : p239-241).

Of course, at first glance, experiencing orgasm during coitus may appear to be a bit late for 'mate choice', since, by the time coitus has occurred, the choice in question has already been made. However, given that, among humans, most sexual intercourse is non-reproductive and does not result in conception, the theory is not altogether implausible.

On this view, the very factors which Symons views as suggesting female orgasm is non-adaptive – such as the relative difficultly of stimulating female orgasm during vaginal sex – are positive evidence of its adaptive function in carefully discriminating between suitors/lovers to determine their desirability as father for a woman's offspring.

Nevertheless, at least according to the stringent criteria set out by George C Williams in his classic 'Adaptation and Natural Selection', as well as the more general principle of parsimony (or 'Occam's Razor'), the case for female orgasm as an adaptation remains unproven (see also Sherman 1989;  Case Of The Female Orgasm ).

Out-of-Date?
Much of Symons' work is dedicated to challenging the views of Sixties ethologists such as Desmond Morris. Although scientifically now somewhat obsolete, Morris's work still retains popular resonance and therefore this aspect of Symons's work is not entirely devoid of contemporary relevance.

In place of Morris's rather idyllic notion that humans are a naturally monogamous 'pair-bonding' species with a mating system analogous to that of gibbons, Symons advocates an approach rooted in the individual-level (or gene-level) selection championed by figures such as George C Williams and Richard Dawkins.

This leads to some decidedly cynical conclusions regarding the true nature of sexual and romantic relations among humans.

For example, Symons argues that it is adaptive for men to be less sexually attracted to their wives than they are to other women – because they are themselves liable to bear the cost of raising offspring born to their wives but not those born to other women with whom they mate (e.g. those mated to other males).

Another cynical conclusion is that the primary emotion underlying the institution of marriage, both cross-culturally and in our society, is neither love nor lust, but rather male sexual jealousy and proprietariness (p123). Marriage, then, is an institution borne not of love, but of male sexual jealousy and the behaviour known to biologists as 'mate-guarding'.

Meanwhile, in his excellent chapter on 'Copulation as a Female Service', Symons suggests that most heterosexual romantic relationships may be in some respects analogous to prostitution.

'The Evolution of Human Sexuality' is also out-of-date in a more serious respect. Namely, it fails to incorporate the vast amount of empirical research on human sexuality which has been conducted since the first publication of his work.

For a book first published thirty years ago, this is inevitable – not least because much of this empirical research was inspired by Symons' own ideas and specifically designed to test theories formulated in this very work.

In addition, potentially important new factors in human reproductive behaviour that even Symons did not foresee have been identified, for example role of levels of fluctuating asymmetry functioning as a criterion for, or at least correlate of, physical attractiveness.

For an updated discussion of the evolutionary psychology of human sexual behaviour, complete with the latest empirical data, readers should consult the latest edition of David Buss's  The Evolution Of Desire .

In contrast, in support of his theories Symons relies largely on classical literary insight, anecdote and, most importantly, a review of the ethnographic record.

However, this focus in some respects ensures that the work remains of more than merely of historical interest.

After all, one of the more legitimate criticisms recent research in evolutionary psychology is that it is insufficiently cross-cultural and, with several notable exceptions (e.g. Buss 1989), relies excessively on research conducted among undergraduate samples at Western universities.

Given costs and practicalities, this is inevitable. Undergraduates in the very institution where one is employed to research and teach represent, of course, a 'convenience sample'. However, for a field that aspires to understand a human nature presumed to be universal, such a method of sampling is highly problematic.

'The Evolution of Human Sexuality' therefore retains its importance for two reasons. Not only is it the founding work of modern evolutionary psychological research into sexual behaviour, and hence of importance in the history of the field and of science, but it also remains of value to this day for the cross-cultural and ethnographic evidence it marshals in support of its conclusions.
________

References
Bellis & Baker (1990). 'Do females promote sperm competition?: Data for humans'. Anim. Behav., 40, 997-999
Bobrow & Bailey (2001). 'Is male homosexuality maintained via kin selection?' Evolution and Human Behavior, 22, 361-368
Bogaert & Hershberger (1999) 'The relation between sexual orientation and penile size'. Archives of Sexual Behavior 1999 Jun;28(3):213-21.
Buss (1989). 'Sex differences in human mate preferences: Evolutionary hypotheses tested in 37 cultures'. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 12, 1-49
Ellis & Symons (1990), 'Sex Differences in Sexual Fantasy: an Evolutionary Psychological Approach', The Journal of Sex Research 27 (4): 527-555
Havlíček et al (2016) 'Men's preferences for women's breast size and shape in four cultures' Evolution and Human Behavior 38(2): 217–226
Kenrick & Keefe (1992). 'Age preferences in mates reflect sex differences in human reproductive strategies'. Behavioral and Brain Sciences, 15, 75-133.
Kruger et al (2003) 'Proper and Dark Heroes as Dads and Cads' Human Nature 14(3) 305-317
Manning et al (1997) 'Breast asymmetry and phenotypic quality in women' Ethology and Sociobiology 18(4): 223–236
Miller (1998). 'How mate choice shaped human nature: A review of sexual selection and human evolution'. In C. Crawford & D. Krebs (Eds.), Handbook of evolutionary psychology: Ideas, issues, and applications (pp. 87-129). Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum
Møller et al (1995) 'Breast asymmetry, sexual selection, and human reproductive success' Ethology and Sociobiology 16(3): 207-219
Palmer (1991) 'Human Rape: Adaptation or By-Product?' Journal of Sex Research 28(3):365-386
Penton-Voak et al (1999) 'Menstrual cycle alters face preferences', Nature 399 741-2.
Puts (2010) 'Beauty and the Beast: Mechanisms of Sexual Selection in Humans', Evolution and Human Behavior 31 157-175
Salmon (2004) 'The Pornography Debate: What Sex Differences in Erotica Can Tell Us About Human Sexuality' In Evolutionary Psychology, Public Policy and Personal Decisions (London: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates, 2004)
Sherman (1989) 'The clitoris debate and levels of analysis', Animal Behaviour, 37, 697-8.
Read less
33 people found this helpful
--
Matthew T. Carpenter
5.0 out of 5 stars Amazing work.
Reviewed in the United States on January 31, 2016
Verified Purchase
I was floored by the breadth of this book. Amazing work.
4 people found this helpful
--
raisinhead
5.0 out of 5 stars A good starting pointing
Reviewed in the United States on December 31, 2013
Verified Purchase
Not as up update now since publication was some years ago but an excellent framework of biology and concepts to develop further understanding.
One person found this helpful
--
James E Rogers
5.0 out of 5 stars Scientific view of sexuality
Reviewed in the United States on February 8, 2018
Verified Purchase
A classic on the origins of sexuality. Thought provoking insights in the human condition. Regaining Control: Winning the Battle Against Sexually Abusive Behavior
--
William S. Johnson
5.0 out of 5 stars A Classic Work in the Study of Human Sexuality
Reviewed in the United States on February 4, 2014
Verified Purchase
This book is a bit dated, but Symons is an excellent researcher and author. This is a very useful resource.
2 people found this helpful
--
John Engelman
5.0 out of 5 stars The Sociobiology of Human Sexuality
Reviewed in the United States on March 28, 2015
In his Preface to “The Evolution of Human Sexuality” Donald Symons writes, “my discussion of sex differences in sexuality is not intended to have social policy implications.”

I suspect Professor Symons wrote that to stay on the invite lists of faculty dinner parties at the University of California, Santa Barbara where he taught. An additional concession to political correctness appears in Chapter Two, “Evolution and Human Nature,” where he writes, “It is generally agreed that insufficient time has elapsed since the invention of agriculture 10,000 years ago for significant changes to have occurred in human gene pools.”

In “The 10,000 Year Explosion: How Civilization Accelerated Human Evolution,” University of Utah professors Gregory Cochran and Henry Harpending present a convincing argument to the contrary.
http://www.amazon.com/The-000-Year-Explosion-Civilization/dp/0465020429

Symons goes on to write, “Certainly there is no evidence that living hunter/gatherer peoples – whose ancestors presumably were all hunter/gatherers – are less well adapted genetically to agricultural or to modern technological societies than are peoples whose ancestors lived in such societies since their inception.”

The evidence is dangerous to list, so I won’t.

Fortunately, most of the book does not read like that. The Evolution of Human Sexuality is an important contribution to sociobiology. Sociobiology is the scientific study of the biological foundations for animal and particularly human behavior. Sociobiology assumes that human nature exists, and that while it is flexible, it is not infinitely malleable.

Symons’ main argument is that the sexuality of human males and females is fundamentally different. The differences evolved because sexual behavior that increased the number of descendants a person of one sex would be likely to have reduced the number of descendants a person of the other sex would be likely to have.

Females of most reptiles and all amphibians and fish lay many eggs, but do nothing to raise their young. Females of mammals and birds do raise their young. However, compared with males they are limited in the number of young they can give birth to. Males are only limited by the number of females they can impregnate.

It is in the biological interest of females to be impregnated by what sociobiologists call “the fittest available male.” In practice that usually means the best fighter. However, females of many species need the help of a male to help them raise their young. In these species females settle for a dependable dad who will take his fatherly responsibilities seriously. These species are strictly or at least usually monogamous.

With most bird species one partner needs to tend the eggs and the chicks while the other looks for food. The other will return to the nest; they will trade places; the first will look for food. An interesting exception is the chicken. Males and females of most bird species look so similar that it is difficult to tell what their sex is. Roosters and hens look so dissimilar that they might as well belong to different species.

They not only look different. Consider the practice of cock fighting. You might expect male eagles to engage in more interesting fights than male chickens. However, eagles are monogamous, so the males do not fight.

If a male only gets one female he has little reason to fight for her, because another will be along shortly. If the difference between fighting and winning and avoiding combat means the difference between copulating with several females and not copulating at all, fighting is worth the risk.

Most mammals are polygamous. It is easy to tell the difference between a stag and a doe, or a bull and a cow, or a ram and a ewe. Foxes and gibbons are monogamous. Males and females are the same size, and of the same appearance.

Birds and mammals range in behavior from geese, which mate for life, to elephant seals. Male elephant seals are several times as large as females. Most males never copulate. Males who win the fights may have as many as 200 pups.

Humans are between these extremes, although closer to the geese. During most of human evolution most humans have practiced monogamy. Among Paleolithic hunting bands every man gets one wife. The head man, and the best hunters and warriors get two or three, but seldom more. The reason every man can have one wife, while exceptional men get more than their share is because a large number of men get killed fighting men of other hunting bands, and hunting animals that fight back and win.

Most modern civilizations outlaw polygamy. Nevertheless, men are more likely to commit adultery than women. Successful married men are often allowed to have mistresses, unless they are elected President of the United States.

Many men would like to be promiscuous. Few men are able to be that way. Most women could be promiscuous. Few women want to be. Throughout human evolution a man who had sex with many women and who refused to take care of any of them could expect some of them to live. A woman who had sex with many man would gain nothing, and lose much. Few men would want to help her raise her children, because few or none of those children would be theirs.

Several Paleolithic peoples did not understand the relationship between sexual intercourse and pregnancy until it was explained to them. Sociobiology assumes that animals behave in ways that increase the number of descendants they have, even though they do not understand that relationship.

Symons mentions the example of groupies. Although most women do not enjoy casual sex, there are women who enjoy casual sex with celebrities. Even though few groupies want to have children with the rock stars they fornicate with after rock concerts, their instincts tell them that these rock stars have good genes, and that if they have a child by one of them, the child will get many of those good genes too.

Although many male stars encourage groupies, I have not read of any female rock stars that encouraged groupies, not even Madonna.

Virtually all the customers of the sex industry are men, and as many sniggering teenage boys as can get away with it. Symons mentions the magazine Playgirl. This was founded in 1973, and modeled itself after Playboy. Playgirl features nude photos of men. However, most of those who buy the magazine are male homosexuals.

Symons presents the example of homosexuals as what he calls “test cases” of his thesis. Male and female heterosexuals need to compromise between what they want and what they are able to get. Most male heterosexuals are not celebrities. They do not get any groupies. Many female heterosexuals feel the need to kiss a few ugly frogs before they find their prince.

With homosexuals, no compromise is necessary. The market for nude photos of men and male prostitutes consists almost exclusively of male homosexuals. Gay bars for male homosexuals are places where male homosexuals find sex partners they may never see again. Male homosexuals tend to be much more promiscuous than male heterosexuals.

Lesbians are seldom promiscuous. They like to pair off with other lesbians in fairly durable monogamous relationships. Bars that attract lesbians are not places to find casual sex partners. They are places to take friends. Lesbians are seldom interested in nude photographs of women. They seldom patronize female prostitutes.

The female equivalent to pornography is women’s romance novels. These are sometimes sexually explicit, but little or no interest is shown in the genitals of the men. The heroines are frequently virgins. If the heroines have sex with many men it is unsatisfying. The stories end happily when the heroine marries the fittest available male, who remains faithful to her.

The implications of The Evolution of Human Sexuality, which the author deftly eludes in his preface in order to avoid embarrassing conversations at fashionable dinner parties, are hostile to the sexual revolution. They are not necessarily political. In a country like the United States there is little the government can do to influence sexual behavior. This is why the religious right has not been able to restore the ethos of the 1950’s when a popular song expressed the consensus with the lyrics, “Love and marriage go together like a horse and carriage.”

Professor Symons does write, “modern women’s sexual emancipation may have the effect of making some men reluctant to form durable heterosexual relationships.”

He also writes of a survey in Redbook magazine, “Women who had had sexual intercourse by age fifteen tended to be more sexually experimental and to have had a greater number of partners before marriage, but also to be more unhappy sexually, and to orgasm less frequently.”

In “The Hidden Agenda of the Political Mind” Robert Kurzban reports, using the U.S. General Social Survey (GSS) as his authority, that when both partners are virgins when they get married, there is a 85 percent chance of them staying married.
http://www.amazon.com/Hidden-Agenda-Political-Mind-Self-Interest/product-reviews/0691161119/ref=cm_cr_dp_see_all_btm?ie=UTF8&showViewpoints=1&sortBy=bySubmissionDateDescending

The national average for divorce is about 50 percent. This has increased dramatically since love and marriage ceased to go together like a horse and carriage. So has the illegitimacy rate. Children raised to adulthood by both parents living together in matrimony tend to do much better in life than children raised in other situations.

Sociobiology assumes that human nature has been shaped by Darwinian evolution. Most Christian conservatives reject Darwinian evolution. Nevertheless, the implications of sociobiology are congenial to the values of the religious right.
Read less
4 people found this helpful
Helpful
Report abuse
See all reviews
Top reviews from other countries
Amazon Customer
1.0 out of 5 stars It's for scholars, not for common readers
Reviewed in Australia on March 2, 2020
Verified Purchase
Don't buy it unless you have the patience to read research papers
Report abuse

Byoung Kweon Kim 빌게이츠가 기후위기에 대해 정말 얘기한건 무엇이었을까?

Facebook

소장 at 정의당 부설 정의정책연구소
Former 협치자문관 at 서울시 (Seoul)
Former 이사 at 사회혁신공간 "There"
Former 센터장 at 서울혁신센터
Former 부원장 at 새로운사회를여는연구원
Studies 사회학 at 성공회대학교 대학원
Studied Economics at 서강대학교 Sogang University
Studied Chemistry at 연세대학교 (Yonsei University)



[정책과 책소개 8] 빌게이츠가 기후위기에 대해 정말 얘기한건 무엇이었을까?
...
김영사가 번역해서 영문과 거의 동시 출판한<기후재앙을 피하는 법> 책의 공개를 앞두고, 조선일보가 빌게이츠와의 인터뷰를 미리 내보내서 논란이 되었다. 그 제목은 '탄소제로 하려면 원전필요'였다.
졸지에 빌게이츠가 핵발전으로 기후위기를 돌파하자는 주창자가 되었다. 그런데 정말 궁금한 것이 있다. 조선일보나 다른 미디어는 빌게이츠만큼 '2050탄소제로'에 대해 확실한 목표의식이 있을까?
둘째 공평하게 평가한다고 해도, 빌 게이츠는 현재의 핵발전도 아니고 그의 회사가 개발하는 차세대 핵발전을 강조하고 있기는 하지만, 그것보다 수십배 재생에너지 투자와 조속한 확대를 주장하고 있고, 전기차로의 전환(수소차는 거의 언급안됨)도 강조하는데, 이런 대목들에는 공감이 있을까? 심지어 빌게이츠는 강력한 탄소가격설정도 찬성하는데?
이걸 다 빼고 맥락없이 빌게이츠는 기후위기 솔루션으로 핵발전을 생각한다는 식으로 마구 언론에 퍼뜨리니, 빌게이츠가 매우 억울해 할것 같다. 빌게이츠는 조선일보나 유사언론사에 명예훼손으로 소송을 해야 하는것 아닐까 싶다. ㅎ
/


그럼 빌게이츠의 책은 어떤책인가? 읽을만 한가? 개인적으로는 읽을만 했고 도움도 되었다. 나의 종합적 평가는 이렇다. 2050년 탄소제로에 도달하기 위해서 '기술적으로 도전할 수 있는 모든 것에 대한 성실한 탐구'를 하고 있는 책이란 점이다.
그 와중에, 그리 길지 않는 꼭지로서 그가 2008년에 창업한 테라파워서가 실험실 수준에서 작업중인 '진행파 원자로'를 적용해야 한다고 주장하고 있을 뿐이다
(핵발전이 탄소제로사회를 위한 대안이 왜 안되는지는 최근 조천호 박사께서 언론에 명확히 정리하셔서 충분히 논박되었다고 생각하니 한번 보기바란다.)
그는 탄소없는 전기생산을 어떻게 해야 할지, 철강/시멘트/플라스틱에서 탄소배출 제로를 어떻게 달성할지, 축산등에서 탄소배출 제로에 어떻게 도달할지, 교통과 운송에서, 냉방과 난방에서 각각 탄소배출을 어떻게 줄일지 주로 기술적 측면에서 해법들과 도전과제를 나열한다. 이 대목들은 꽤 살펴볼 만한 것들이 많다. 세계 최고의 부자가 세계최고 전문가들을 불러서 들었던 요약 브리핑을 간결하게 정리해놓았으니 뭐.
정작 그의 기술적 검토에서 핵발전보다 더 나쁜 것이 있다. 그가 여전히 '지구공학'에 대한 집착에서 벗어나지 않고 있다는 것이다. 지구의 대기 성층권에 아황산 에어로졸을 뿌려 태양빛을 차단하는 방식 같은 '지구공학'이 "비상시를 위한 최첨단 기술이라고 생각할 수 있다. 이것이 성공한다고 해서 온실가스 저감이라는 우리의 책임이 면피되는 것은 아니지만, 그래도 제대로 행동을 할 수 있을 때까지 시간을 벌어줄 수 있다"고 믿는 것이다.
/
그리고 내가 생각하는 빌게이츠의 가장 취약한 문제는, 그가 도모하는 기술적 해법의 일람표에 있지는 않다.
  • 그는 기술적 '혁신'은 엄청나게 다양하게 접근하면서도,
  • 정작 제도와 생활의 혁신에 대해서는 지나가다가 잠깐잠깐 얘기하는 정도에 그치고 있다는 것이다.
쉽게 말하면, 빌게이트는 지금 부자로 살고 있는 패턴을 가급적 안바꾸고, 자신이 투자하는 기업들을 포함해서 대부분의 기업들도 하던 수익추구를 그대로 하고, 현재 제도적으로 자원 배분의 불평등이 극심한 상황에서도 사회적 재분배 등을 생각하지 않고 있는 것이다.
조금 과장되게 표현하면, 지금까지 누리던 삶과 제도를 그대로 두고, 단지 화석연료를 쏙 빼버리고 여기에 재생에너지등을 교체하고 싶은 것이다. 그러니 당연히 핵발전도 끼여들고 그러지 않을까 싶다.
화석위에 쌓아올린 문명은 화석연료를 빼버리면 상당한 정도로 바뀔 수 밖에 없다는 사실을 충분히 받아들이지 않는다는 것이다. 그리고 당장은 기후위기가 엄청난 도전과제이지만, 이를 포함해서 9가지 지구의한계를 다 고려했을때 빌게이츠가 발상하는 방식이 여전히 유지될지도 충분히 생각하고 있는 것 같지 않다.
----------
의외로, 그는 정부와 정부의 정책의 역할을 매우 크게 생각하고 있다. 고무적이다.
그는 탄소제로를 위한 연구개발 투자를 5배 이상 늘릴 것을 포함해서 정부가 해야 할 7가지 과제를 요약하는데 나름 의미있게 정리해다고 생각한다.
특히 다음과 같은 혁신국가적 관점은 조선일보가 진지하게 검토해보면 도움이 될텐데.
"국방부의 초기 투자로 인터넷과 마이크로칩이 탄생해 개인용 컴퓨터 혁명이 가능해졌고, 이는 기술산업의 혁신으로 이어졌다. 청정에너지 산업에서도 비슷한 일이 일어날 수 있다. 제로탄소 시멘트나 철강, 또는 온실가스 배출이 없는 액체연료를 만들 사람을 기다리는 수십억 달러 규모의 시장이 있다."
이 대목에서 빌게이츠의 약점은 또 하나가 드러난다. 그가 정부의 미션을 비교적 상세히 일별하는 것에 비하면, (민간)기업의 과제는 맨끝에 조금 지적하다가 그만둔다는 것이다.
사실 스스로가 기업가 출신이라면, 탄소배출 제로를 위해서 기업이 해야 할 일이 훨씬 더 많다는 것을 인정해야 할텐데. 세금부터 똑바로 내는걸 포함해서. 이 대목에서도 그는 자신이 살던 패턴을 그다지 바꾸고 싶어하지 않는다.
....
*그래서 결론은? 위의 분명한 약점이 있다는 것을 알면 좋은 정보가 의외로 많다고 나는 생각한다. ㅎ
May be an image of text that says "HDMI Battery ter BILL Power 기후재앙을 빌 게이츠 GATES AVOID CLIMATE A HOW 피하는 법 TO 우리가가진솔루선관우리에게 우리가가진 DISASTER 이엽 솔루션과 빌게이츠 우리에게 김민주 옮김 김영사 필요한 돌파구"
강길모, Hyuk Bom Kwon and 101 others
2 comments
19 shares
Like
Share

Comments

  • 그런데 이 책의 번역에서 86쪽 '태양열'이라고 한 부
2 comments
Byoung Kweon Kim
그런데 이 책의 번역에서 86쪽 '태양열'이라고 한 부분은 영어 원문은 그냥 solar 이던데, 이건 태양광쪽으로 해석해야 하는거 아닌가?
그리고 325쪽 '공정한 전환'이라고 번역한 부분....언제부터 just transition이 정의로운 전환이 아니라 공정한 전환이 되었대? ㅠ
 · 6 w

Sunkyo Kim
Byoung Kweon Kim 저는 둘다로 보고 태양에너지라 번역하는데...역자가 에너지를 잘 모르는 분 같습니다